Print-to-Speech or Speech-to-Print? That is the Question Redux

Blog Banner
26 July, 2025

speech-to-print phonics

phonics

Blast from the Past: This piece first appeared on June 4, 2022, and was re-released on July 26, 2025. I continue to feel strongly that speech-to-print is the best way to go when it comes to beginning phonics instruction, yet as this entry admits – that is not yet proven. That hasn’t changed over the past few years, and yet data continue to accumulate on that side of the ledger. For example, Yan, et al. (2024) demonstrate the importance of a neurological speech-to-print convergence in reading, something that suggests the potential value of such instructional approaches. Likewise, there is more evidence concerning the relationship between speech-to-print processing and word reading (Vandervelden, M., & Siegel, 2001). Other studies show the sensibility of the speech-to-print sequence in early literacy development (Rowe, Piestrzynski, Hadd, & Reiter, 2024). I’ve touched the piece up a bit, adding some history, but the point remains: we don’t know which approach is best, but until someone proves one to be better than the other, I’d bet on speech-to-print as the starting point.

Teacher question:

I know you typically don’t talk about specific programs, but I really would like to know your thoughts. I had always wanted more training in a structured literacy program/approach. I always thought Wilson, and specifically OG approaches, were the gold standards. More recently, I began reading about programs labeled as “speech to print.” Proponents of speech to print methods claim it is much faster to teach kids to read (and spell) than OG based approaches. Is there research to support this? Are these studies comparing programs based on OG (that mainly follow a more print to speech approach) and programs that are more specifically speech to print? Thank you!

 Shanahan response:

You’re right. I rarely comment on specific programs. But I’m happy to discuss program research or how consistent with the research a specific aspect of a program might be.

Let’s start with the claim that Orton-Gillingham (OG) style programs are the “gold standard.”
To me, a gold standard for an instructional program would be an approach that consistently resulted in positive learning outcomes and did better than competing methods. This outperformance would be demonstrated by direct research comparisons, or by meta-analyses summarizing a bunch of disparate but related comparisons. Basically, a gold standard approach would result in more learning.  

If OG was the gold standard, then it would reliably do better than other explicit decoding approaches in teaching kids to read.

Back in National Reading Panel (NRP, 2000) days, we analyzed evaluations of phonics instruction across 38 experimental and quasi-experimental studies – considering 18 different phonics curriculum sequences. Our conclusion? Phonics was a valuable ingredient in literacy teaching, and programs with explicit systematic phonics tended to outperform those that did not.

What about different types of phonics teaching? We made some of those comparisons, too. For instance, synthetic phonics (reading words from individual letters and sounds) resulted in higher average effects over analytic phonics (focused on syllables, morphemes, and use of known words as analogies), but this difference wasn’t statistically significant. Basically, there was no real difference in the effectiveness of synthetic and analytic phonics.

We didn’t compare individual phonics programs with each other because there were usually only one or two studies of most programs.

One exception to this was OG. There were enough studies of OG programs to allow the computation of a meaningful estimate of effectiveness. Nevertheless, we didn’t do that. I still think that was a good choice. I suspect those results would be a bit misleading. When OG failed, it was usually being delivered to severely disabled populations, including with kids so disabled that they were hospitalized. It is fair to say that OG didn’t do especially well with such kids. But none of the comparable programs were ever evaluated under those circumstances. There is no reason to think they would have done any better.

Over the past 25 years, more research has accumulated, and OG now has its very own meta-analysis (Stevens, Austin, Moore, Scammacca, Boucher, & Vaughn, 2021). That study found OG to be effective but with rather modest benefits – a lower degree of effectiveness than was reported for the average phonics program studied in the NRP report.

So much for being a gold standard!

Orton-Gillingham procedures are no more effective than any other explicit systematic phonics instruction – despite the religious fervor of some of its advocates.

Not surprising, those true believers argue against the data:

“They didn’t look at the right version of OG.”

“I do it a little differently than others and it really works well for my kids.”

“The newer trainers aren’t as good as the past ones, so they probably studied teachers who weren’t well trained.”

Those things are not more (or less) true of OG than any other phonics approach. If it’s so hard to find a potent version, then why would we expect it to be more effective than anything else?

There aren’t direct comparisons of OG with other phonics approaches, but generally it looks like it works about as well as any of them (and, sometimes, not as well).

That brings us to a second point – the one about speech-to-print instructional approaches.

I’ve oft grumbled about the lack of evaluation of individual features of complex instructional programs. Research may affirm the benefits of a multicomponent program without revealing which of its ingredients were active.

You’d think with all the interest in phonics there would be many such studies exploring the implications of sound tracing, analytic/synthetic approaches, grapheme-phoneme sequences, inclusion of morphological analysis, decodable text, emphasis on consistency versus flexibility, print-to-speech/speech-to-print approaches, dosage variation, decoding for older students, and so on.

Unfortunately, there are surprisingly few such research studies. Comparisons of print-to-speech and speech-to-print programs being one especially unfortunate area of neglect. We have some relevant research information about differences between those approaches – just nothing definitive.

Basically, speech-to-print approaches encourage students initially to perceive their language sounds prior to focusing their attention on the letters and spellings. As children become aware of the phonemic distinctions between words, then these perceived sounds are linked to their visual or print representations. Beyond that speech sound beginning, these teaching methods encourage much spelling and writing activity – actions that require students to think about what letters represent the language sounds and word pronunciations, rather than solely focusing on sounding out written words.

Phonics programs have usually emphasized print-to-speech. Kids are taught to identify letters, to link sounds to those letters, and then to sound out words by sounding each letter. Think about it.  That instructional sequence is identical to the sequence readers must go through during reading: look at the letters and use that information to generate a phonological representation.  

It seems reasonable to teach students explicitly what we eventually want them to do. That isn’t always the case, however.  To be effective a curriculum design does not necessarily have to mirror the intended outcome quite this accurately. Learning to read and reading are not the same thing, and one doesn’t necessarily benefit from mirroring the other.

Perhaps the opposite – starting with phonemes and pronunciations and then connecting those to letters and printed words – might be a good idea. It’s possible that trying to spell and write words does more to enhance phonemic awareness and it may somehow make the phonology more prominent or easy to perceive (Wasowicz, 2021).

The possibility that kids might benefit more from speech-to-print than from print-to-speech first started to appear in the 1910s – in the work of Ernest Horn (1919). He explored the idea that spelling instruction and practice might benefit reading. Later, this notion gained purchase when Helen Murphy (1943) discovered that the ability to auditorily discriminate the phonemes in words was a powerful predictor of later reading progress, and she and her advisor, Donald Durrell, built this concept into their beginning reading program (Durrell, Sullivan & Murphy, 1945), a program that eventually morphed into Speech to Print Phonics (Durrell & Murphy, 1964).  

When Jeanne Chall (1967) published her landmark review of phonics research, she concluded that those programs that included spelling, writing, and/dictation did better than those that did not.

I followed up on that in my reading-writing relationship research in the 1980s. I found spelling and decoding to be closely related, even when a lot of other variables were available to suck up the variance (Shanahan, 1984). Later, Ginger Berninger and her colleagues improved on that with even more ambitious efforts with the same results (Berninger, Abbott, Abbott, Graham, & Richards, 2002).

Marilyn Adams (1990) and Linnea Ehri (1997) both theorized on that possibility, and Steve Graham reported a meta-analysis that found spelling instruction improved reading – most likely because of its contribution to decoding (Graham & Santangelo, 2014).

But that evidence is all indirect. It suggests the value of the approach. It doesn’t prove it.

Not everyone agrees with my conclusion. Louisa Moats (1998, 2005, 2010), for example, has several publications that treat this as a settled matter, concluding that speech-to-print is best. There is a difference between what I believe and what I know. There is a preponderance of evidence on that side of the argument, but I’m not satisfied that it is yet proven.

Perhaps the closest thing we have to a direct test of the proposition is a meta-analysis of 11 studies (Weisler & Mathes, 2011). It concluded that instruction that integrated encoding into decoding instruction led to significantly higher reading achievement. Still not the strongest evidence – because it combined investigations that compared encoding with decoding (Christensen & Bowey, 2005) along with those that compared encoding instruction with things like extra math lessons (Graham, Harris, & Chorzempa, 2002).

No matter where the final evidence comes down, it should be remembered that at some point – any decoding program will necessarily shift its focus to print-to-speech, since that is what we do in reading. Until then, it is fair to conclude that there are real benefits to be derived from activities like explicit early instruction in phonemic awareness, invented spelling encouragement, explicit spelling instruction, word construction from sounds, and so on. These kinds of speech-to-print activities increase learning.

My advice: get a phonics program that includes such activities or layer them into a traditional print-to-speech program (including OG).

References

Adams, M. (1990) Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(1), 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500104

Chall, J.S. (1967). Reading: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Durrell, D. D., Sullivan, H. B., and Murphy, H. A. (1945). Building word power. Yonkers on Hudson: World Book Co.

Durrell, D. & Murphy, H. A. (1964). Speech to print phonics. New York: Harcourt Brace and Jovanovich.

Ehri, L. C. (1997). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell (pp. 237-269). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Graham, S., & Santangelo, T. (2014). Does spelling instruction make students better spellers, readers, and writers? A meta-analytic review. Reading and Writing, 27, 1703-1743. DOI:10.1007/s11145-014-9517-0

Horn, E. (1919). Principles of method in teaching spelling as derived from scientific investigation. Teachers College Record, 20(7), 52-77. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/016146811902000703 

Moats, L.C. (1998). Teaching decoding. American Educator, 22(1), 1-9.

Moats, L. C. (2005). How spelling supports reading and why it is more regular and predictable than you may think. American Educator, 29(4), 12-22, 42-43.

Moats, L. C. (2010). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

Murphy, Helen A. (1943). An evaluation of the effect of specific training in auditory and visual discrimination of beginning reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University

National Reading Panel (U.S.) & National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U.S.). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read : an evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Rowe, D. W., Piestrzynski, L., Hadd, A. R. and Reiter, J. W. (2024). Writing as a path to the alphabetic principle: How preschoolers learn that their own writing represents speech. Reading Research Quarterly, 59: 32-56. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.526

Shanahan, T. (1984). Nature of the reading-writing relation: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 466–477.

Stevens, E.A., Austin, C., Moore, C., Scammacca, N., Boucher, A.N., & Vaughn, S. (2021). Current state of the evidence: Examining the effects of Orton-Gillingham reading interventions for students with or at risk for word-level reading disabilities. Exceptional Children, 87(4), 397-417.

Vandervelden, M., & Siegel, L. (2001). Phonological processing in written word learning: Assessment for children who use argumentative and alternative communication. Argumentative and Alternative Communication, 17(1), 37-51. https://doi.org/10.1080/aac.17.1.37.51

Wasowicz, J. (2021). A speech-to-print approach to teaching reading. LDA Bulletin, 53(2), 10-18.

Weiser, B., & Mathes, P. (2011). Using encoding instruction to improve the reading and spelling performances of elementary students at risk for literacy difficulties: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 17-200.

Yan, X., Yang, F., Feng, G., Li, H., Su, H., Liu, X., Wu, Y. Hua, J., & Cao, F. (2024). Reading disability is characterized by reduced print-speech convergence. Child Development, 95(6), 1982-1999. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdeve.14134

Comments on Earlier Version

More Past Comments

LISTEN TO MORE: Shanahan On Literacy Podcast

Comments

See what others have to say about this topic.

What Are your thoughts?

Leave me a comment and I would like to have a discussion with you!

Comment *
Name*
Email*
Website
Jenny Chew Aug 01, 2025 03:50 PM

I’m probably of a similar vintage to Gaynor! I’ve lived in the UK since 1970, but was brought up in South Africa, and have clear memories of an approach that started by teaching 26 letters and just one sound for each. It was letter-driven in that sense, and also in the sense that there was great emphasis on decoding by looking at letters from left to right, saying sounds for them and blending the sounds, starting with vc and cvc words. We were then taught a good range of digraphs, so we knew that there were more sounds than could be represented by single letters, though the total number of sounds was never mentioned. We also learned to spell by segmenting spoken words and writing a letter/digraph etc. for each sound, but we also had to learn spellings from a list, which I think helped us to realise that encoding was harder than decoding. While we were still in primary school we were taught prefixes and suffixes, their functions, and rules for adding them to base words. This all worked very well – there was no reading failure among my contemporaries, and when I later started my secondary-school teaching career there, I encountered no serious reading problems. Spelling was also good – not always perfect, but nearly always logical.

I think it makes good sense to teach both print-to-speech (decoding) and speech-to-print (encoding) routines, but I also think it makes sense to teach at least a few letters and sounds as a first step, so that children know what we mean by ‘sounds’ in this context. Those isolated sounds are not the same as the co-articulated sounds of normal speech, and beginners find it hard to segment spoken cvc words into the smallest units, as we see in videos advocating that they do this before learning the letters for the sounds. I also have a problem with the way that sound-to-print often presents decoding as the reading back of words just encoded. This is not really decoding, as the spoken form of the word is already known, so the child is not working it out from the letters as would have to happen in any real act of decoding.

The approach which has been officially approved in England since 2007 has been to start by spending a few days teaching 4-6 letters and sounds in both directions (see the letter, say the sound – hear the sound, write or point to the letter), then, as more correspondences are taught, teaching decoding for a week or so before teaching encoding, with the two proceeding in tandem from then on.

Natalie Bremner Jul 30, 2025 02:53 PM

Responding because the earlier version of this post is what sparked my interest in exploring Speech to Print programs.

Of those available, EBLI is the program I opted to explore. It seems that this particular program is an effective, efficient approach to helping students improve their reading, writing, & spelling skills at any age.

It is the efficiency of EBLI that drew me in. It’s the efficiency that parents & teachers, myself included, search for. It’s the efficiency that our students so desperately need.

Nora Chahbazi, EBLI’s founder, shared on a podcast that her goal with EBLI is to put a fence at the top of a cliff to prevent the need for ambulances at the bottom.

As a classroom teacher & tutor with students beyond K-2nd grade, my concern is for those students dangling off the sides as well as those already at the bottom.

Sure, some may say this paints an awfully unpleasant scene, but in my experience this scene pales in comparison to what those who are struggling in school experience day in & day out.

I don’t think anyone reading this blog denies the importance or need for effective reading instruction. However, David Chalk’s interviews on YouTube speak to the personal devastation that is undeniable. They speak directly about a program that can make a life changing impact at any age.

I’m not saying other speech to print programs are not effective. I’m not even saying print to speech programs are not effective. It’s the efficiency that EBLI can offer that sparked my interest. If my child is hanging off a cliff, I’m going with effective & efficient.

I’ll certainly update my post in time. At this point, this particular program genuinely seems worth exploring.

Natalie Bremner Jul 30, 2025 02:54 PM

For context, here’s the comment by
Jennifer Newman that inspired me to look into EBLI…

“I believe there is still some confusion regarding how we are defining “speech to print” (S2P) and “print to speech” (P2S) approaches to phonics. Most S2P approaches (Phono-Graphix, EBLI, Sounds-Write, Reading Simplified, Abecedarian, etc) organize their instruction around the 40+ phonemes of English and teach students the logic of our alphabetic code - sounds can be represented by 1-4 letters, some letters represent more than one sound), along with explicit practice segmenting, blending, and manipulating phonemes during encoding and decoding. As Diane McGuinness explained so well, all P2S phonics programs teach the code “backwards” which obscures it’s logic and necessitates teaching over 200 letter-sound correspondences, rules and exceptions to rules, and syllable types based on print characteristics rather than spoken language. This takes much more time and effort and many of our most struggling readers never achieve proficiency.”

Gale Morrison Jul 30, 2025 04:29 PM

I think you need to clarify what you mean on "invented spelling encouragement" because that leads directly to "spelling doesn't matter it's author's voice" and "we have spell check for that."

Timothy Shanahan Jul 30, 2025 05:34 PM

Gale--
Years of experience with young children tells me just the opposite. Young kids are trying to understand the spelling system, being able to try it out to examine its logic advantages kids and does not give them the idea that spelling doesn't matter. If it did, their spelling wouldn't consistently improve. By thinking about the sounds and trying to figure out which sounds match which letters/letter combinations in which parts of the words, they end up with better phonemic awareness, better decoding skills, and, yep, better spelling ability. The mistake that many people make is to think that such activity takes the place of spelling lessons, it does not.

tim

Gale Morrison Jul 30, 2025 05:46 PM

I'm talking about the teacher disposition and understanding that comes with telling them "you should encourage invented spelling" this becomes "never correct it" very easily. And Calkins and TCRWP took this snippet of sound advice and turned it into training teachers and Ed school profs who train teachers to say "students invented spelling is good." Then in late elementary and secondary the school and teachers are telling students "spelling doesn't matter."

This DOES in fact turn students into poor spellers, and/or keep them poor spellers. I have seen this myself in dozens of classrooms with thousands of kids. This is rampant in balanced literacy. Meaning it's what's going on in 80-90% of our classrooms right now.

Timothy Shanahan Jul 30, 2025 06:21 PM

Gail--
Those are interesting claims -- but they neither match the research or personal experience with this going back more than 50 years now.

tim

Miriam Giskin Jul 31, 2025 11:51 AM

Great article. I was at the Harvard Reading Lab when Jeanne Chall was the director so I greatly appreciate the historical perspective. I understand that Dr. Ehri recently stated; “In terms of my view of morphology recently, in considering the work of Sue Hegland and
Peter Bowers, I realized that orthographic mapping can be conceptualized as recruiting units of the morphographic system to connect morphemic units in spelling of words to their meaning and syntactic functions. This bonding is equally important.” I would be interested to know your thoughts on that and where you think the research regarding morphology in reading instruction should next focus.

Gaynor Aug 01, 2025 04:45 AM

I hope my comments are helpful to the discussion . Having been involved in helping out in the teaching of remedial students with phonics for 50 plus years , I have decided I like the methods of traditional phonics as was the method used in NZ 1930-50. Reading instruction began with learning all the sounds of the alphabet , with extra emphasis on the short vowels. Simultaneously with this was learning 30 of the commonest 'sight' words which were either irregular in their spelling or had more advanced phonemes. These words played no part in phonic instruction but were for the sole purpose of allowing the child to start reading simple stories which included these common words and all the 150 or so short vowel three letter words. Predictable and flexibility in reading right from the beginning with a large vocabulary of all the three letter words. No child moved onto learning other phonemes until they could read and spell all the words they read . If they struggled they would be given extra practice with phonograms (word families like the -at or - ap or -et family) . Some children , but not all seemed to need this extra variation. One practical activity was home made flash cards with eg a picture of a man on one side and the word on the other . These were useful for spelling and reading and a child could use them as an individual activity , for homework , creating the two piles of right and wrong , and repeating until all cards were correctly read or spelled. The word families were listed at the back of the reading book for chanting or spelling as well as the three letter words jumbled up in lists.
Since I had a child of mine with severe dysgraphia, I would have liked him to have spent much more time on practicing the printing of the alphabet as a four year old , as well as just phoneme awareness activities. Saying the sound as he repeatedly printed them. Hyperactivity . is a nasty disorder but it seems is linked to dysgraphia . There was a good half hour spent on handwriting , every day ,earlier last century . Here is another traditional activity that needs to be reclaimed. Little kids enjoy repetition, I have observed in their games and other activities.

I am unashamedly a traditionalist but value Timothy's consciousness in reading research . Curiously little of it contradicts traditional methods.

Jennifer Newman Aug 04, 2025 06:10 PM

I appreciate your decision to update this post, Dr. Shanahan, as this is a topic that deserves further attention. Yet, at the risk of sounding like a broken record, I feel compelled to point out again that the term "speech-to-print" continues to have different meanings for different people, and I see that this is muddying the exploration of this topic.

Based on what you've described in your post, it sounds like you are characterizing speech-to-print as a way to initiate reading instruction by first focusing on developing phonemic awareness through oral exercises, and subsequently teaching the written code through encoding activities that involve moving from sound to print.

However, as I commented in your 2022 publication of this post, many people in the field use the label "speech-to-print" to refer to approaches that follow a "sound-to-print" instructional framework or paradigm in which phonemes are the anchors for helping students create mental "file folders" for learning the various spellings of English phonemes. Many, but not all, are based on the work of Diane McGuinness, who referred to this framework as Linguistic Phonics. These McGuinness-based approaches go beyond using encoding as an activity; they explicitly teach students the logic of how our code represents phonemes in print and emphasize explicit teaching of straightforward procedures for both encoding and decoding that draw upon this logic. Rather than teaching oral phonemic awareness as a separate activity, students develop their phonemic awareness skills while simultaneously learning the alphabetic principle through the basic 1:1 mappings of speech sounds to print for both encoding and decoding in the context of real words and reading and writing simple texts.

Along with flipping the orientation and organization of instruction, the methods, instructional language, and pacing used in Linguistic Phonics approaches differ substantially from those used in traditional phonics, OG-based teaching, and even some approaches that use the label Speech-to-Print, where explicit teaching of declarative knowledge (e.g., digraphs, blends, r-controlled sounds, vowel teams, trick words, syllable types, rules and exceptions, etc.) is put forth as the most effective and equitable route to creating proficient readers and writers. Unlike Linguistic Phonics, these approaches lack a cohesive framework and instead organize their instruction according to a print-based instructional scope and sequence that guides teachers, but largely leaves students on their own to figure out the logic and organization of our written code.

So, while your recommendation to "layer" speech-to-print activities into a traditional print-to-speech phonics program might make sense using your conceptualization of speech-to-print, it would not make any sense to mix and match approaches for someone who understands speech-to-print to mean a wholly different instructional paradigm.

Timothy Shanahan Aug 04, 2025 06:20 PM

Jennifer--

That's all well and good -- you are looking at it right and I am looking at it wrong. However, I would point out the need to rely on empirical data rather than Diane McGuinness' opinions. You may claim superiority for Linguistic Phonics -- I'll wait for the data that support those claims. As far as I can tell, it does no better than any other phonics program (even if it creates file folders in the kids' heads).

tim

Jennifer Newman Aug 05, 2025 12:54 AM

Thank you for your response, Dr. Shanahan. I agree wholeheartedly that empirical evidence should guide instructional decisions. While more large-scale studies would certainly be welcome, several peer-reviewed and program evaluation studies support the efficacy of Linguistic Phonics approaches grounded in the work of Diane McGuinness. For example:

Case, Philpot, & Walker (2023) conducted a longitudinal study on the Sounds-Write program, showing strong outcomes across multiple years.

Denton et al. (2006) evaluated an intensive intervention that included Phono-Graphix and found meaningful gains.

Endress et al. (2007) published a program evaluation of Phono-Graphix with students with disabilities, again showing promising results.

Simos et al. (2007) used brain imaging to show that phoneme-based instruction like Phono-Graphix shifted activation to typical language regions in struggling readers.

McGuinness, McGuinness, & McGuinness (1996) reported early findings from the development of Phono-Graphix.

In Essentials of Assessing, Preventing, and Overcoming Reading Difficulties (2015), David Kilpatrick reviewed intervention research and noted that traditional phonics programs tended to yield only moderate standard score gains, while programs like Phono-Graphix showed some of the largest—suggesting they deserve further investigation.

In addition to the published research, teachers and interventionists who use Linguistic Phonics across all tiers of instruction continue to report strong classroom outcomes. Stephanie Bolton, for example, has shared compelling case studies and data in recent blog posts on the Goyen Foundation’s The Science of Reading Classroom Substack page.

Just to clarify: my intent in commenting on your post wasn’t to argue for the superiority of Linguistic Phonics, but to note that the term speech-to-print is being used in different ways, which understandably creates confusion. I also hoped to briefly explain how approaches like Linguistic Phonics differ methodologically from more traditional phonics instruction. The goal was simply to add clarity to the discussion and invite further exploration of these distinctions.

Comments

Print-to-Speech or Speech-to-Print? That is the Question Redux

12 comments

One of the world’s premier literacy educators.

He studies reading and writing across all ages and abilities. Feel free to contact him.

Timothy Shanahan is one of the world’s premier literacy educators. He studies the teaching of reading and writing across all ages and abilities. He was inducted to the Reading Hall of Fame in 2007, and is a former first-grade teacher.  Read more

60 E Monroe St #6001
CHICAGO, Illinois 60603-2760
Subscribe